














Sign
up for our Free newsletter
|
|
|
|
[Eckersley, R. 2001,
That’s all well and good...’, The
Sydney Morning Herald, 30 June, Spectrum,
pp.2-3.]
That’s
all well and good...
...but
if society is becoming more prosperous, why do young people seem
less happy?
Richard
Eckersley looks at how science is
struggling to sift the data from day-to-day reality.
Late last year I asked Year
11 students at a private boys school whether they’d ever thought
about the meaning or purpose of life.
Almost all raised their hands.
I asked if they had ever felt that life seemed meaningless or
pointless. Quite a few
–between a third and a half – indicated they had.
Most admitted to having some sort of spiritual or religious
belief, but none volunteered a description of that belief.
The boys’ responses
don’t fit neatly into the popular images of young people today –
either the portraits of happy, hedonistic teenagers and young
adults, revelling in the freedoms and opportunities of contemporary
life, or the pictures of distress and disillusion amidst material
excess, social inequity and spiritual dessication.
What is emerging from the
scientific research into well-being are the subtleties, complexities
and depths of the human psyche, and of the personal, social and
spiritual ties that lie behind our health and happiness.
At the same time, science is straining to define and
differentiate these things. Our
politics and economics have barely begun to come to grips with them.
If we want to assess the
state of society, a good place to begin is with young people and how
well they are faring. There
is growing evidence that developmental stages and transition points
in life, from before birth to adolescence, are crucial to adult
health and well-being. What
happens at these times matters for life, and it makes the young
susceptible to the effects of social failing and disruption.
However, research is
throwing up more troubling questions than providing definitive
answers; findings are fragmented and contradictory.
Some surveys and commentaries indicate the young are thriving
in the postmodern world of rapid change and uncertainty, others that
they are anxious and apprehensive.
Differing views can reflect
different disciplinary frameworks, different political ideologies,
and selective or partial use of research findings. Attempts
to lay blame get confused with efforts to explain.
Some analyses focus on marginalised youth, others (such as in
the current debate about boys’ education) on gender.
Many commentaries on young people are framed in generational
terms: conflict and competition between Baby Boomers and Gen X;
periodic ‘moral panics’ by adults about youth; or historical
cycles.
Judith Bessant and Rob
Watts, two Melbourne youth researchers, say that concerns about
young people as ‘victims of change’ or ‘sources of misrule’
are a recurring historical myth unsupported by empirical evidence.
They say they are arguing ‘against some of the widespread
generalisations made about young people as problems or victims’,
but their thesis goes well beyond this, to the point of denying that
the myth has any basis in reality.
This view is also reflected
in some recent US writing, with the added dimension that if there
has been a youth crisis, then we are over the worst, and things are
now improving (there is some evidence of this in Australia, but not
yet much). Mike Males
argues in his 1999 book, Framing
Youth: 10 Myths About the Next Generation, that American
teenagers today are better behaved than adults today, than today's
adults when they were young, and than adults have a right to expect
given the way young people are treated.
Rates of serious crime, drug abuse, self-destructive
behaviour and school failure among youth today are lower than they
were 20 years ago.
David Brooks, author of an
influential 2000 analysis of contemporary America, Bobos
in Paradise: The New Upper Class and How They Got There, takes
the upbeat appraisal further in a recent essay in The
Atlantic Monthly. Drawing
mainly on interviews with students at Princeton and other Ivy League
universities, he presents an approving image of happy, incredibly
hard-working conformists who don’t have a rebellious or alienated
bone in their bodies: respectful, obedient, responsible, clean,
generous, bright and good- natured.
Brooks admits he is writing
about an elite, but he nevertheless states that they are ‘not
entirely unlike’ other young Americans.
Princeton reflects America, he says, and ‘in most ways it
reflects the best of America’.
Both Males and Brooks mention the work of historians William
Strauss and Neil Howe, who in a 1997 book, The
Fourth Turning: An American Prophecy, argue that history runs in
cycles of 80-100 years, with each cycle having four turnings, and
each turning being associated with a different generational type.
The post-war Baby Boomers
are classic prophets, indulged and ‘spirited’; Generation X,
born during the second half of the 60s and the 70s, are typical
nomads, neglected and ‘bad’; today’s teens, the Millennials,
born in the 80s, are the next heroes, protected and ‘good’.
The fourth generation in the current cycle, yet to be born,
are the artists, suffocated and ‘placid’.
Brooks notes Howe and Strauss surveyed young people for their
latest book, Millennials
Rising, published last year, and found them to be generally
hard-working, cheerful, earnest and deferential.
The positive view is
supported by recent suggestions that even a disturbing trend like
rising youth suicide may not mean what it seems to mean – rising
unhappiness. Jim
Barber, professor of social welfare at Flinders University, recently
compared youth suicide rates with adolescent self-esteem, school
adjustment and social adjustment in seven countries, both Asian and
Western. He found that
the higher the level of self-esteem and adjustment, the higher the
male suicide rate.
I examined associations
between youth suicide rates in up to 21 developed nations and a wide
range of social, economic and cultural characteristics, and found
that male suicide rates were highest in the most individualistic
countries. The more
personal freedom and control over their lives young people felt they
had, for example, the higher the suicide rate.
Given other positive
correlations between individualism and happiness and life
satisfaction, my results, like Barber’s, seem to suggest that
suicide is higher in happier societies and, presumably, rises as
life gets better. Possible
explanations include that suicidal behaviour increases when unhappy
people have fewer outside sources on which to blame their misery;
that the greater happiness of most increases the misery of the few;
or that social changes such as increasing individualism are good for
the majority but bad for a minority.
Barber
says his findings suggest that when vulnerable young people perceive
those around them to be better off than they are, their distress is
magnified and their susceptibility to suicide is increased.
‘If you are a depressed, unhappy kid in a country where you
are surrounded by kids who are happy and well-adjusted, then you
have a double problem – you are depressed and you are isolated as
well.’ While these
explanations are plausible, I doubt they are right.
A detailed analysis of these
perspectives is beyond the scope of this article.
However, a core element is the notion that the vast majority
of young people are okay and doing well, and that those in trouble
are a small, discrete minority.
The opening article in the current, ‘youth’ issue of VicHealth
Letter, published by the Victorian Health Promotion Foundation,
begins: ‘Most young people, an estimated 90 per cent, live
healthy, happy lives and make the transition into adulthood
smoothly’. Consistent
with this positive interpretation, surveys do show that about nine
in ten young Australian say they are healthy, happy and satisfied
with their lives.
Yet a recent Victorian
study found 25-40 per cent of students aged 11-18 experienced in the
previous 6 months feelings of depression, worries about weight,
worries about self-confidence, troubles sleeping, and not having
enough energy. A survey
of students aged 11-15 in 28 Western countries found that while the
great majority (over 90 per cent in many nations) reported feeling
healthy and happy, significant minorities (reaching majorities for
some countries, ages and complaints) also admitted to ‘feeling
low’ and having headaches and stomach aches at least once a week,
and to feeling tired most days of the week.
Another Victorian study of
year 7, 9 and 11 students showed 23 per cent of girls and 12 per
cent of boys reported ‘high levels of depressive symptoms’.
In a large Queensland survey, 52 per cent of 15-24-year-olds
had experienced at least one episode of depression in their lives
(defined as ‘a period of feeling sad, blue or depressed that
lasted for two weeks or more’), and either 34 per cent or 18 per
cent were currently depressed, depending on the ‘cut-off’ point
in the depression scale used in the research.
A study of Queensland
university undergraduates found almost two thirds admitted to some
degree of suicidal thoughts or behaviour in the previous 12 months,
at least to the extent of feeling that ‘life just isn’t worth
living’, or that ‘life is so bad I feel like giving up’.
Almost a quarter admitted to suicide-related behaviour,
including telling someone they wanted to kill themselves or
attempting it.
A large survey of women’s
health in Australia has found that young women reported the highest
levels of stress, were often tired, and were over-concerned with
their weight and body shape. A
long-term study of four representative cohorts of young Australians
suggests declining well-being, based on a nine-item subjective
well-being index.
These findings are
mirrored in public perceptions of life for young people today.
When, two years ago, I polled almost 100 teachers in ACT
colleges (years 11-12) on whether they thought the social and
emotional well-being of young people in Australia was getting better
or worse, 81 per cent said it was getting worse.
In a 1999 US survey of how life in America today compared
with the 1950s, teenagers were one of only two groups (the other
being farmers) for whom a clear majority of Americans (56 per cent)
thought life today was worse. Life
for children also rated poorly, with only 46 per cent saying it was
better today.
The point about these
comparisons is to show that the picture of young people’s
well-being can depend crucially on the questions asked or the
indicators used. More
specifically, they show measures of self-reported health, happiness
and satisfaction do not present an adequate or accurate account of
health and well-being.
Overall, the evidence shows
the prevalence of social and psychological problems has increased
among young people and is higher than in older age groups.
It does not support the view that there is a small group of
troubled youth clearly segregated from the mainstream, or majority,
of young people who are happy, healthy and thriving.
The distinctions between
them are often of degrees; there are gradients of disturbance,
distress and discomfort that include a large minority of young
people today, perhaps even a majority at some time in their lives.
Regardless of whether we look at crime, depression, drug use,
or suicidal thought and behaviour, we find these gradients in the
severity and prevalence of youth problems.
Nor does the evidence
indicate that those at greatest risk to their health and their lives
are all located, or even heavily concentrated, among the most
materially disadvantaged. While,
generally speaking, there are socio-economic gradients in health -
worse health at the lower end of the social scale, better at the top
- the relationship is not consistent and clear-cut, and varies
according to the cause of death and gender.
Let me be clear about what I
am saying here. It is
not to give the impression of universal, serious pathology, or to
‘medicalise’ or ‘problematise’ common human emotions and
experiences. It is to
show that there are links between even extreme personal distress and
more prevalent, but less serious, suffering, and that the sources of
these conditions can be traced to defining qualities of our
societies. In other
words, these sources are social and pervasive as well as personal
and specific, and problems must be addressed at both levels.
Youth suicide represents the tip of a large iceberg of
suffering, not a tiny island of misery in an ocean of happiness.
My interest in these issues
is primarily not to identify why one individual and not another has
a problem or disorder, which can then be treated, but to explore the
social significance of population patterns and trends.
‘There is but one truly serious philosophical problem, and
that is suicide,’ the French writer, Albert Camus, wrote.
‘Judging whether life is, or is not worth living amounts to
answering the fundamental question of philosophy.’
So what lies behind the
social and psychological problems associated with being young these
days? We don’t really
know. Disadvantage,
poverty and unemployment may play a role but, as I’ve already
indicated, their importance is unclear and they don’t appear to
explain the trends in these problems.
Changes in family life, including increased conflict, abuse
and neglect, and in adolescent transitions are likely factors.
In a major international
review, two British researchers, Michael Rutter, a child and
adolescent psychiatrist, and David Smith, a criminologist, call for
further investigation of the theory that shifts in moral concepts
and values are among the causes of increased psychosocial disorder.
They note, in particular, ‘the shift towards
individualistic values, the increasing emphasis on self-realisation
and fulfilment, and the consequent rise in expectations’.
British sociologists, Andy
Furlong and Fred Cartmel, say that ‘the processes of
individualisation, coupled with the stress which develops out of
uncertain transitional outcomes, have implications for the health of
all young people’. They
note the increased sources of stress ‘which stem from the
unpredictable nature of life in high modernity’.
These include the ongoing sense of doubt, the heightened
sense of insecurity, the increased feelings of risk and uncertainty,
and the lack of clear frames of reference that mark young people’s
world today.
While traditional forms of
inequality remain, they say, even young people from privileged
social backgrounds worry about failure and the uncertainty
surrounding their future. Conversely,
those from disadvantaged backgrounds may feel that the risks they
face are personal and individual rather than structural and
collective.
Individualism could impact
on youth suicide and other problems through its effect on specific
social institutions and functions, such as the family and
child-rearing. In my
analysis, for example, both youth suicide and individualism were
negatively correlated with sense of parental duty (it is
‘parents’ duty is to do their best for their children even at
the expense of their own well-being’) – that is, suicide and
personal autonomy were greater in those countries where a smaller
proportion of the parental generation agreed with this statement.
Individualism’s effects
may go further than this, however.
Western societies – and some more than others – may be
taking this trait to the point where it can become more broadly
dysfunctional, to both society and the individual.
In other words, these societies are promoting a cultural norm
of autonomy that is unrealistic, unattainable or otherwise
inappropriate. They
project images and raise expectations of individual freedom, choice
and opportunity, and of the happiness these qualities are supposed
to deliver, which are increasingly at odds with human needs and
social realities.
Brooks’ interviews with
Princeton students casts an interesting light on these issues.
He sees them as the products of an era of parental
protection, prosperity and peace.
They are ‘the most honed and supervised generation in human
history’, he says. In
contrast to the freedoms granted young people in the 1960s and
1970s, this is a group whose members have spent most of their lives
in structured, adult-organised activities.
‘The kids have looked upon this order and decided that
it’s good’.
Brooks does qualify his
positive view. He notes
the growth in medicating children with disruptive behaviour with
Ritalin and similar drugs, and the rise in the proportion of college
freshmen who say they feel ‘overwhelmed’.
The rules are growing stricter by the year.
The students appear to be instructed on just about every
aspect of life, except character and virtue; and they are lively
conversationalists on just about any topic, except moral argument.
Perhaps the busyness and the striving are to compensate for
what is missing, he suggests.
The students are highly
goal-oriented. Activities
are rarely an end in themselves, but the means for self-improvement,
resume-building – for climbing, step by step, ‘the continual
stairway of advancement’. There
is little time or energy for serious relationships, it seems, or for
national politics and crusades. ‘People are too busy to get
involved in larger issues,’ a student journalist tells Brooks.
‘When I think of all that I have to keep up with, I’m relieved
there are no bigger compelling causes’.
Jean Twenge, an American
psychologist, recently examined survey data from 1952 to 1993 and
found large, linear increases in anxiety and neuroticism in children
and college students in the US.
‘The average American child in the 1980s reported more
anxiety than child psychiatric patients in the 1950s,’ she notes.
Twenge ascribes the
increased anxiety to low social connectedness and high environmental
threat (fears of violent crime, AIDS, nuclear war etc), both of
which she says are linked to increasing individualism.
She says there may have been improvements in some areas since
the early 1990s, but not in others.
The past year has seen a surge in public and professional
concern in the US over the harmful pressures on children associated
with ‘hyper-parenting’ and increasingly organised, structured
lives – a trend also apparent in Australia.
Brooks spoke to those who
have thrived on this regimen. But
even these high-flyers will, sooner or later (and especially when
they stumble on the stairway), wonder what they are striving so hard
to achieve, and whether it is worth the effort.
They will ask what their lives mean.
In the lives of these
privileged, clever students – just as in the lives of the poor,
dispossessed and despairing - we see reflected the values and
priorities of our societies. Much
of the research literature, the contradictions notwithstanding,
suggests these values and priorities are the very opposite of what
promotes personal and social well-being.
Still, there are grounds for
optimism. While science
may never give us clear-cut recipes for social improvement, it is
contributing to a growing willingness to question and discuss what,
all things considered, makes a better life.
It is better that we obtain imperfect knowledge about the
important issues of our times than precise answers to what are, in
the overall scheme of things, trivial questions.
Richard
Eckersley is a fellow at the National Centre for Epidemiology and
Population Health, the Australian National University. Email:
richard.eckersley@anu.edu.au
Note:
This version may differ from the published article because of
editorial changes.
|
|
|
|