Taxonomies
of Mind
Marcus
Bussey
To arrive at an holistic understanding of the
cognitive development of the child we must find a model to describe
the multifaceted nature of humanity.
Such a model would need to have the flexibility to accomodate
the recent philosophical shifts that have stressed the relative
nature of ‘reality’, a postmodern sensibility, while still
containing within it the strength and vigour to promote a synthetic
and satisfying vision of the evolution of human consciousness.
When faced with the wide ranging descriptions
of this evolution the truth of the Enlightenment philosopher
Helvetius’ observation that, “Man is a model exposed to the view
of different artists; every one surveys it from some point of view,
no one from every point of view”[1],
is brought home. Over the century there have been many such
‘artists’ seeking to describe the cognitive development of the
child, the most influential being Jean Piaget.
The model he arrived at has held sway in the educational
world for over fifty years now.
It is convincing in both its detail and in its convenience of
application but it is flawed and incomplete as it does not allow for
deeper forms of understanding, for cultural distinctions and for
wide ranging variations in the ages at which children can enter into
his categories.
Piaget’s model has seduced the great majority
of western educators because it is so elegant.
There is a sense of the self evident truth of it yet when we
look at the thinking of L.S.Vygotsky, Maria Montessori, Howard
Gardner, Noam Chomsky, or Kieran Egan, to name but a few, we are
also struck by a sense of validity which we would embrace to get a
broader, more effectively useful image of the child.
Further more, if we expose our schema to the
disturbing yet deeply invigorating critiques of the critical
thinking of Jurgen Habermas[2]
who questions the political role that images of knowledge have both
societally and institutionaly; and of the disruptive analysise of
the postmodern thinkers Michel Foucault[3]
and Jean-Francois Lyotard[4]
who push our conception of knowledge out beyond the comfort zone of
complaceny into the deeper waters of being where structure lies
hidden beneath the surface of things like a reef waiting to scuttle
the boats of the unsuspecting.
Then we find that the need for a model to ‘make sense of it
all’, to, as the political scientist Sohail Inayatullah puts it,
“reinstate the vertical in social analysis, ie from postmodern
relativism to global ethics”[5],
becomes all the more pressing.
Such a model by accomodating layers of
consciousness, moving the debate from the superficial “to the
deeper and marginal”, while still allowing for a range of
“transformative actions” at both the level of policy and at the
level of individual agency[6]
will have relevance where it really counts: in the lives of people.
Causal
Layered Analysis
Causal layered analysis (CLA), an emergent
futures tool developed by Inayatullah, is a model which offers us
the flexibility to read the theories of cognitive development in a
coherent way as it allows for previously exclusive modes of
understanding to inhabit the same theoretical space.
There is no doubt that this method could be used
superficially to gloss over major differences in orientation and
perspective, but that is not the intention of its author.
Inayatullah places this method in the tradition
of critical futures which ‘is less concerned with disinterest, as
in the empirical, or with creating mutual understanding, as in the
interpretive, but with creating distance from current categories.”[7]
As such it is what is unique or different about each category
which is to be highlit, rather than what is similar - it is
interested in making heterogeneity work for us in giving meaning to
the complexity of existence rather than in homogeneity which seeks
to find one voice for all occasions.
Essentially the latter category is doomed to failure while
the former is always at risk of dissolution.
Thus it is the attempt, the ever renewable process of
creating meaning, which becomes priveleged rather than one
authoritative discourse.
There are four layers of analysis within the
CLA framework.
·
Layer 1:
“Litany”, a superficial and disconnected space inhabited by
popular unreflective slogans. It
deals with quantitative trends and problems and is the domain of the
mass media and party politics.
·
Layer 2:
“Social”, offers some in depth analysis at a social,
historical, economic and cultural level.
This is the domain of most academic work and of those working
in policy institutes.
·
Layer 3:
“Structural”, looks at the deeper issues of structure, discourse
and world view. Here we
understand that discourse and the language of discourse are
complicit in framing issues - ie they constitute the issues under
examination.
·
Layer 4:
“Myth and Metaphor”, here we find the deep stories that
define and frame our emotional responses to issues, the unconscious
dimension.[8]
Scanning
the field
Current text books on cognitive psychology will
offer us lists of some of the major categories to dominate the field
this century. Piaget’s
model always gets the most coverage with less space being offered to
Vygotsky. There will be
mention of some alternatives to this dominant constructivist model,
for instance Lee and Gupta,
in an excellent introducory psychology text, allow a few pages for
the nativist approach of Chomsky and Jerry Fodor.
The value of this examination is seen to be in knowing that
there is an alternative view, “that contrasts with that of
Piaget”[9]
who offers a general as opposed to a domain specific intepretation
of cognition. They go
on to examine the multiple intelligence theory of Howard Garner,
then briefly describe the behaviourists as “the scientific
descendents of John Locke” and look equally briefly at the social
interactionism of Jerome Bruner.
As a text for students they are offering no
more than a current map for influential or dominat knowledge forms
and derivative, marginal ones.
Theorists like Kieran Egan, who offers a recapitulationist
interpretation of cognition, do not even get a mention.
Other texts such as Bjorklund’s
Children's Thinking[10]
offer a similar break down, but the dominance of Piaget’s
thinking is acknowledge by him receiving a chapter to himself.
The key theories to dominate the field are:
·
Constructivism
- child initiates their own learning which expands through an active
dialogue with the environment;
·
Behaviourism
- child is an empty vessle who learns through coming in contact with
their environment;
·
Nativist
and associationists -
child recieves a particular level and pattern of genetically
determined abilities, these remain stable over time;
·
Recapitulationists
- the child’s development follows the evolution of humanity from
the primordial savage mind to the enlightened human one, meaning
making occurs through moving from the whole to the specific;
·
Social
Interactionism - mind is shaped by culture; mental life unfolds
with the aid of cultural codes and traditions.
Putting
the Method to work
CLA offers some interesting insights when
applied to these categories. What
Inayatulaah described as the vertical within this method is a debt
to Foucault, the French postmodernist, who developed the metaphor of
‘archaeology’ as a way of digging beneath the surface of things
to uncover the roots of a cultural or social practice like ‘psychologising’.
A clearly cultural practice which has sought to draw to
itself the credibility of science to add weight to its assertions
about humanity.
The model also has a more conventional,
horizontal aspect which allows us to place models within the current
academic orientations of left, right and centre; subjective or
objective; positivist, humanist, or neo-humanist; and so on.
It goes without saying that such orientations owe everything
to their own ‘archaeologies’ and the root metaphors which
inspire them.
Put simply, CLA has a clear ontological thrust
which asks the unasked question: What metaphor lies at the root of
each ‘isms’ description of human nature?
This is fundamental because as Helvetius reminds us, all
researchers are artists working with their own subjective image of
what is primary about the human condition.
Thus for Piaget the child makes themselves, sui
generis, through a dialogue with their environment.
This self-making is an instinctual drive which is invariant,
following a set pattern of stages working through sensori motor,
pre-operational, concrete operational to formal operational.
Fellow constructivist Vygotsky, also believes in the self
generation of cognition within the child, but he places much more
emphasis on the role of the environment and significant others in
the child’s life.
Piaget’s model is elegant and has its roots
in the Enlightenment vision of ‘Man’ as the measure of all
things. Vygotsky on the
other hand is indebted to Marxist analysis of humanity as evolving
through clashes with material existence.
Humanity as a whole, not as a conglomerate of individuals.
To pursue Helvetius’ artist metaphor and take a great work
of art to represent each thinker’s representation of the essence
of humanity, Piaget’s Layer 4, unacknowledged and unconscious,
image might be Jacob Epstein’s sinewy representation of Ecce
Homo. Vygotsky’s
mythic, Layer 4, roots are better described with reference to the
muscular imagry of Ferdinand Leger, as captured in a painting like The
Builders, in which the men are part of the steel and machinery
they use.
Now working horizintaly we can see that
Piaget’s model fits quite comfortably within the domain of the
second layer as a penetrating attempt at mapping human consciousness
which is, in his mind, reasonable, and fundamentally rational.
The second layer of CLA being fundamentally the domain of
empiricism and analysis. Vygotsky also inhabits this layer but moves
a little deeper, acknowldging the force of social structures in the
formation of human consciousness.
His is not as elegant an analysis, not offering neat
categories that can describe and validate a child’s progress, but
he does allow for society to have a voice in the formation of the
child. He is more aware
of the power of structure in the formation of identity.
More
Digging, more Metaphors
Interestingly behaviourism also has its roots
in the Enlightenment but rather than looking to Rousseau for
inspiration it looks to Locke, describing the child as a tabula
rasa. The text
books do not give much time to the simple behaviourism of Pavlov or
Skinner. Yet for the
purposes of this excersise it is helpful to bring them in.
Behaviourism, by describing the human as a machine that is to
be programmed, through repeated stimulus and response, by their
interaction with the environment; by seeing humanity as the sum of
our experiences, can be placed within the first layer as an example
of unreflective empiricism based upon a shallow conception of human
consciousness.[11]
A metaphor for this conception of humanity
might be found in the works of Michelangelo, who left us the
enigmatic Prisoners,
human figures trapped in stone, emerging but forever stuck, being
unfinished. They remind
us of Condillac’s description of humanity as “a statue
constructed internally like ourselves, and animated by a mind which
as yet has no ideas of any kind.”[12]
For Condillac, as for his behaviourist descendents, the
senses were the key to learning, and humanity was trapped by them in
an often unforgiving environment.
The psychological upshot of such a bleak proposition was that
the mind needed to be conditioned to the ‘good’ though exposure
to positive influences.
The nativist position of Chomsky moves towards
the level of structure, what he calls a ‘Universal Grammar’,
(Layer 3) because it sees human meaning making as resulting
from our ‘languaging’ our environment[13].
Chomsky sees a direct link between language and consiousness,
he points to how governments and the media simplify language (simple
language = simple mind) in order to supress critique[14].
Thus his method is disruptive of categories and more
virulently political than previously examined methods.
Other nativists are not as difficult as Chomsky.
For instance, Fodor has a much less complex position.
Essentially he argues “that we are all born with identical
representational and computational systems, which are genetically
prestructured to allow us to make sense of the world in which humans
evolved.”[15]
A visual metaphor to situate Chomsky’s
thinking in Layer 4 could be taken from Michelangelo again.
The famous creation image of Jehova and Man in the Sistine
Chapel, underwrit with the phrase: “In the beginning was the
word”, gives voice to the poweful Judeo-Christian quality of his
thinking. Fodor on the
other hand makes claim to nothing so grand, and might best be
characterised by a piece from the modernist painter Peter Halley
such as Two Cells with
Circulating Conduit.[16]
Egan
and Bruner
Fodor occupies a Layer 2 position while Chomsky
moves between Layers 2 and 3, offering a more problematic
description of the development of consciousness, meaning and
language. Kieran Egan,
on the other hand, offers a revitalised version of the
recapitulationist theory[17]
which holds that human beings develop consciousness through a series
of stages, roughly analogous to the stages of human evolution, that
move them from a mythic undifferentiated sensibility, through
romantic and philosophic consciousness to the ironic perspective
that characterises full mastery of the adult world. He is interested
in the role of the mythopoetic in the formation of meaning, while
pointing to deep structures in our thinking that respond to story as
a powerful tool in understanding the world[18].
Egan’s method, growing as it does from an
imaginative conception of human consiousness, as opposed to the
empirically seductive conception of the human mind as a mathematical
and predictive organism, reaches more deeply into the layering of
Inayatullah’s analysis. It
is essentially a third layer theory which identifies structure as
the origin of our meaning making.
I feel that the metaphor at the root of Egam’s schema is
more darkly mythic and could well be characterised by Goya’s
highly charged Saturn
Eating His Children, an image that evokes the human struggle
towards perfection, a journey from chaos to order that is derived
from an integration of consciousness through an unending process of
‘retelling’ our story.
Bruner makes a case for a model of cognition
that derives from the mind’s experience of culture.
For him, “Learning, remembering, talking, imagining: all of
them are made possible by participating in a culture.”[19]
Culture, it could be said, provides the language with which
we create our self-image. Like
Chomsky, Bruner’s model moves between the deeper discourse of
structure found in Layer 3 and the more superficial, Layer 2, claim
that human beings only have meaning when in a collective: the
rhetoric of ‘One hand Clapping’.
There is a richness about his description which
draws me to a pictorial metaphor of simplicity and power such a
Matisse’s well known painting Primerva,
in which the circle of dancers celebrate life.
This image seems to capture the central dialogue of the self
with the collective which Bruner holds to be the narrative that both
constructs “a version of ourselves in the world” and provides us
with “models of identity and agency”[20].
Towards
a Taxonomy of Mind
This brief analysis places most of the theories
of cognitive development within the second category with the
behaviourists being closest to the level of litany and with Noam
Chomsky’s nativist position and Jerome Bruner’s social
interactionism being closest to the third layer.
Kieran Egan’s recapitulationist position falls within the
third layer.
I think it is the metaphor’s that underwrite
these various theoretical positions that both provide them with
their essential power as well as explain their current position
within educational circles. They
also provide us with a useful taxonomy for understanding western
conceptions of self, as all these models are heavily culture bound,
while leaving the door open for further enrichment of our images
through the introduction of non-western metaphors.
Lets arrange these Layer 4 images clearly, so
that we can better understand their relationship to the dominant
corporate managerial agenda for education.
This will be helpful for our then looking at possible
alternative visions of the child and cognition.
To further speed up this process the essential qualities of
each metaphor, gained from an application of horizontal placement of
theory within the CLA framework, will be identified, so that their
appeal or rejection can better be understood.
Typology
Metaphor
Quality
Constructivism
- Piaget
Ecce homo
Humanist;Passive
Constructivism - Vygotsky
The Builders
Socialist; Active
Behaviourism -
Pavlov and Skinner
Prisoners Positivist; Passive
Nativist - Chomsky Michelangelo’s Creation
Humanist; Active
Nativist - Fodor Two Cells with
Circulating Conduit Positivist; Passive
Recapitulationists - Egan
Saturn Eating His
Children
Evolutionist; Active
Social
Interactionism - Bruner
Primerva
Humanist;Inter- Active
Insights
into the Managerial Mind
This simple taxonomy throws some light on why
Piaget has come out in front of the ‘pack’ in educational
institutions today, dominated as they are by a managerial ethos.
For a start, any theory that allows agency to individuals and
groups is supressed within a corporate managerial setting.
Such a setting operates on an authoritarian model, is product
rather than process oriented, and has a high regard for clearly
managable categories. Though
temperamentaly suited to the positivism of behaviouralism, this is
currently out of favour because it is seen to ‘lack heart’, and
because behaviouralism itself questions the effects of environment
on learning. Corporate
managerialist environments are not renowned for their plasticity or
humanity, they are therfore not open to question.
Fodor’s nativist theory is so passive as to
not allow for the virtue of education within its paradigm.
The managerial approach is highly attached to its image of
efficacy, thus any claim that suggests that ability is inherent to
the organism is too threatening to its raison
d’etre. Bruner’s
inter-active theory is also highly threatening to the managerial
consciousness which needs the self sustaining individual to be the
owner of their destinys in order to better equip them for effective
action within an economic rationalist environment.
This leaves only Piaget, who offers a
comfortably heroic humanism, in which agency is circumscribed by
stage/developmental sequence, and yet who offers a quietistic image
of humanity as hostage to their own cognitive wiring.
Enough of the oppositional theories are also contained within
Piaget’s theory - social influence, instinctual learning patterns,
imaginative unfoldment, etc... to also allay the unease that might
arise over the excessive focus on purely rational, mathematico-linguistic
process that privelege this way of knowing over others.
Loose
Ends
Using the CLA model as a tool in the analysis
of current models of cognitive development raises many issues.
Foremost amongst these is the question of cultural domination
of a field such as cognition by the west.
When all the dominant models of mind are rooted in western
metaphors there is only limited applicability to educational
process.
It also raises the question of the possibility
of finding descriptions of mind that may take more account of the
deeper layers of consciousness validated by such a model.
The implications are great for theories of education such as
those of Maria Montessori, Rudolf Steiner and Prabhat Rainjan Sarkar
who all offer more deeply spiritual and synthetic visions of the
child and the function of education in human development.
Finally, CLA helps us to see the validity of
multiple positions, removing the need for analysis as the handmaid
of choice, we subscribe to one model or the other, and making room
for synthesis, we take parts from all models, as a goad to action.
Here the function of ‘getting real’ cannot be
underestimated. To
teach, and to be guided by theory, is essentially an activity of
conscious selectivity in which we vigorously engage with theory in
order to better understand our craft and the children and future we
serve.